The @WorldAthletics claims that endogenous T is “the main driver” of this difference of sports . By extension, it also claims that women with T levels in the typical male range have an “insuperable advantage” over women with T in the typical female range.
In this thread I address four myths central to the @WorldAthletics claims.
Myth 1: T is the “master molecule of athleticism.” T’s effect on athletic performance isn’t always positive, as the @WorldAthletics own data on elite women athletes well demonstrates.
Its initial analysis of data from two world championship competitions showed that women with higher T had significantly better performances in only five of 21 events.
Serious methodological problems with the @WorldAthletics paper prompted independent researchers led by @RogerPielkeJr to call for the paper's retraction, and the @WorldAthletics issued a correction. But the corrected version still undermines the regulation.
In three of 11 running events, the lowest T group did better, and the strongest association across all events was the negative association between T and performance in the 100 meters, where lower T athletes ran 5.4 percent faster than the highest T athletes.
In none of the events where high T athletes performed better was the gap greater than 2.9 percent.
The @RogerPielkeJr led independent group requested and obtained a subset of the IAAF data, concluding: “The results of [the IAAF’s first study] are clearly unreliable, and those of [the second study] are of unknown validity,” making it “impossible” to discern
the real relationship, if any, between T and performance. Clearly, though, neither this study nor the broader sports science literature support the IAAF’s claim that targeted athletes “have the same advantages over [other] women as men do over women.”
Many studies across a range of sports show similar mixed relationships between performance and T. Consider a recent analysis of teenage Olympic weightlifters, in which the best predictor of strength was lean body mass, which has a complicated relationship to T.
Among girls, body mass was initially the only significant predictor of weightlifting performance, and T was a predictor of body mass.
But, counterintuitively, once the investigators controlled for the girls’ size, they unmasked a strong negative relationship between T levels and performance: girls with lower T lifted more weight.
The researchers noted that T affects muscle, which is crucial to force, but T also affects breast tissue and fat localization in the lower limbs; the latter may be especially important for certain powerlifting moves. Controlling for body mass, there were norelationships between
any hormones and performance in boys, even though their T levels ranged from 0.5 to 30.2 nanomoles per liter. In short, T (and other steroids) affect multiple body systems, and the relationships sometimes work in a positive synergy to improve performance,
but they sometimes detract from performance.
Myth 2: The best way to see what T does for athletic performance is to compare men and women. It’s simple, some people argue: men have “greater lean body mass (more skeletal muscle and less fat), larger hearts (both in absolute terms and scaled to lean body mass),
higher cardiac outputs, larger hemoglobin mass, larger VO2 max (a person’s ability to take in oxygen), greater glycogen utilization and higher anaerobic capacity”; these all affect athletic performance, and are all affected by T,
so men’s greater athletic performance must be due to their higher average T levels.
This is a series of linked, but not necessarily logically connected propositions. The wide range of physiological and social differences between women and men athletes confound those comparisons.
Even characteristics that are influenced by T are also affected by multiple other factors. They can’t simply be boiled down to T, either in adulthood or during earlier development.
Scientists overwhelmingly prefer within-sex comparisons to answer most questions about the factors influencing sports performance, though sometimes it’s useful to analyze data both within and across sexes.
Emerging research using both types of analysis reveals that some factors long thought to be fundamentally sex- differentiated turn out to hinge on other elements.
For instance, most studies have shown that men have a greater proportion of fast-twitch muscle fibers, a difference traditionally attributed to genetics.
A recent study of elite weightlifters, doi.org/10.1371/journa…
though, found women had as many, or more, fast-twitch fibers as men, and concluded that “athlete caliber and/or years competing in the sport influence [muscle fiber proportion] more than sex per se.”
T also likely has a relationship with fiber type via body mass, but as the teenage weightlifter study shows, the relationship with T and body mass isn’t straightforward.
Simple comparisons of women and men athletes can’t reveal the specific relationships that underlie athletes’ physiologies, and can obscure the recursive, sometimes positive and sometimes -negative relationships with T that are in the mix.
Myth 3: Suppressing an athlete’s T reduces performance, so different T levels between athletes must similarly affect performance.
The @WorldAthletics says it has data showing women athletes’ performance suffers following abrupt and dramatic T suppression. While this may be true, the organization isn’t justified in using this observation to conclude that women with higher T levels
“possess a very clear performance advantage” over their peers, on par with what men typically have over women.
The @WorldAthletics narrative suggests that manipulating T only affects athletically-relevant aspects of function, disregarding the fatigue, sleep disturbance, metabolic changes and other physical problems that accompany significant hormonal disruption.
The drop in performance might be attributable to these many side effects, physiological changes and psychological influences.
Moreover, manipulating T in individuals can’t illuminate how T levels figure in differences across athletes: this confuses intra-individual analysis for inter-individual analysis. There’s not just a logical problem with this conflation, but a data problem.
In individuals, “raising or lowering T will show a relationship with performance,” but in analyses across athletes, often “there is no relationship found with performance.”
One reason studies don’t always find consistent links between T level and physiological variables is that sometimes high T signals that a person isn’t very efficient at using T: the body is producing more precisely to arrive at “typical” function.
Myth 4: These @WorldAthletics DSD regulations are solely about T and performance. Scientific claims are central to this debate, but so is the broader context in which @WorldAthletics officials communicate their beliefs about women’s bodies.
The vehicle for performance differences is supposed to be T but, as the @WorldAthletics has been forced repeatedly to defend the T regulations, it has revealed its concern lies less with the T level than with the source of the T.
The @WorldAthletics has made this concern explicit by narrowing the group of women to whom the regulations apply. Women with polycystic ovarian syndrome (PCOS), the most common reason that women have naturally high T levels, and congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH)
were explicitly excluded from the 2019 regulations even when their levels exceed the threshold, though the IAAF has argued that women with PCOS and CAH derive “advantage” from high T.
Likewise, @WorldAthletics statements highlight sex-atypical chromosomes and gonads, worldathletics.org/news/press-rel…
which functions as a dog whistle to suggest that the targeted women athletes are not “really” women.
Yet this rule was supposed to be different from prior sex testing regulations, precisely because it focused on T rather than on other aspects of sex biology that are variable among women (and men).
Even for those who accept that endogenous T makes an outsized contribution to athletic performance, the defining feature is supposed to be the level of T, not the source of it.
The @WorldAthletics enforcement of gender normativity is also evident in its rebuttal of concerns raised by the World Medical Association and the United Nations, among others, about mandating that healthy athletes undergo medically unnecessary interventions in order to compete.
Rather than viewing the serious and long-term consequences of lowering testosterone as “side effects,” the @WorldAthletics proposes that they are “the desired effects.”
These changes—including reduced muscle and increased fat—are supposed to produce the kind of body that Stéphane Bermon, Director of the @WorldAthletics Health and Science Department,has presented as the “ideal female phenotype” at scientific conferences.
Disregarding women athletes who have resisted these interventions, even to the point of bringing legal challenges against the regulation, the @WorldAthletics insists that these “medications are gender-affirming”and “change their body to better reflect their chosen gender.”
The latter statement insinuates that women athletes who do not willingly modify their bodies to fit @WorldAthletics standards actively “choose” their gender, which deliberately encourages confusion with transgender athletes.
These four myths are subtle yet powerful. Owing to the ubiquity the @WorldAthletics “T Talk,” it can be hard even to recognize them. A jumble of science and folklore, T Talk directs attention away from the most important consequences of the regulations by doing what it does best:
making challenges to “common sense” thinking about T and gender seem antiscience. Meanwhile, the building criticisms of the interventions as medically unnecessary intrusions in women’s health are, unquestionably, based on science.
The unwanted manipulations of women athletes’ bodily integrity also contravene both international human rights law and medical ethics.
The @WorldAthletics has couched its DSD policy as the best of bad options, made in the name of protecting women. But it doesn’t seem to be about protecting women.
It seems to be about protecting a specific idea of what it means to be a woman. About protecting some women, just not the ones who look like Caster Semenya.
We could talk about the medieval-sounding “sex verification test” that Semenya was first forced to undergo in 2009, and how the details of it were murky, and how it’s hard to imagine such a test as anything other than humiliating.
“I have been subjected to unwarranted and invasive scrutiny of the most intimate and private details of my being,” she said at the time.
We could talk about all the prurient, invasive, and frequently racist ways we have talked about Caster Semenya over the past 13 years. “It is clear that she is a woman but maybe not 100 percent,” Pierre Weiss, then-secretary general of the @WorldAthletics , said of her in 2011.
He didn’t specify how it was clear, or whom it was clear to, or what percentage of womanhood he was willing to give her.
Most of all, we could talk about what it means to be a woman. And what it means to insist someone is not a woman. And why Michael Phelps was treated like a marvel, and Caster Semenya is treated like a mutant.
End
A lot of unintended harm happens when people assume a Y chromosome makes a person a boy or a man and the lack of a Y chromosome makes a person a girl or a woman.
It is true that in typical male development, the SRY gene on the tip of the Y chromosome helps to send the embryo down the masculine pathway.
But more than the SRY is needed for sex determination and differentiation; for example, women with CAIS have the SRY gene but lack androgen receptors. In terms of hormone effects on their bodies (including their brains), women with CAIS have had much less “masculinization”
Broken Hill, where the outback meets the rough & tumble world of mining. Founded on the dreams of striking it rich with silver, lead, & zinc, this city has a gritty history that's as tough as its landscape. From dusty miners to underground tunnels, Broken Hill has seen it all.
But, there's more to this dusty outback city than meets the eye! Over the years, Broken Hill has undergone a transformation as sparkly as a freshly polished gem.
Social attitudes have shifted, embracing diversity and inclusivity like never before.
From dusty old mines to rainbow flags proudly waving in the outback sun, Broken Hill has become a shining example of acceptance.
The theory of ‘muscle memory’ has been increasingly discussed in relation to TW athletes. Muscle memory theorizes that muscle retains the capacity to perform tasks it has previously undergone, with suggestions that TW therefore, may retain muscle strength advantages over CW
after transition due to cellular or epigenetic marks retained from prior life exposures to testosterone and myonuclei retention. However, it is important to highlight that this area has not been explored in those undergoing GAHT.
Myonuclei retention plays a potential role in muscle memory, with prior research showing myonuclei numbers are associated with training & T use.
No one would need "Help" if their religion didn't teach them they were broken. No one who is same-sex attracted, or gender diverse is broken. The only people who are broken are those who want to torture others for making them think they're broken.
If trans or gay kids were impressionable regarding their own identity then Conversion Therapy would work. And it doesn’t. So they aren’t!!!!!!
Dr. Swaab a 🌎 renowned neuroscientist (written over 540 papers)makes it clear how sexual orientation & gender identity is already installed in our brains before birth & thus why it is short-sighted to discriminate or judge these aspects of human diversity
https://t.co/KKWO73PXAi
There is no research that shows the effect of T on any individual. XX are generally more sensitive to the effects of T than XY, curvilinear effects as well as great interindividual differences make extrapolation of the effects of specific amounts in any individual impossible.
Many aspects of physique or athletic performance differ between M & F, however, none of these is close to 10-fold, further underscoring the limitations of a straightforward comparison of average M-F differences in athletic performance to average M-F differences in T levels.
Handelsman et al. (2018) note that the lower 95% reference limit for men’s endogenous T is 7.7nmol/L, while the upper 95% reference limit is 29.4nmol/L. But the upper limit men are not any bigger, faster, or stronger than the lower limit men.
Lib & Lab Govt funding priorities across 🇦🇺 have effectively criminalised the consequences of marginalisation & failed to address the causes of offending.
This is why our prisons presently warehouse at record rates Aboriginal men, women & children & the mentally ill.
The 1987–91 Royal Commission into Aboriginal Deaths in Custody reported that:
“The single significant contributing factor to incarceration is the disadvantaged and unequal position of Aboriginal people in Australian society in every way”.
Govt's continue to lavish resources on incarceration and private incarceration operated by foreign owned private companies despite empirical research that re-offending is better addressed by rehabilitation programs, education & vocational training, stable housing & employment.