Here's the power contour plot for this scenario ⬇️
If the reader thinks that interesting effect sizes are LOWER than δ = 0.3, then it's easy to see that the chances of reliably detecting such effects drops pretty quickly.
But let's say a study was designed to reliably detect effects δ ≥ 0.8. In some areas, like psychophysics, true effect sizes of δ≥ 0.8 are plausible. But in most other areas of psych this is quite large and unrealistic.
Let's have a look at this power contour plot ⬇️
Imagine this was presented with a paper in a field where true effect sizes of δ≥ 0.8 are unlikely. It's very easy to see that you can't reliably detect more realistic effect sizes and that larger sample sizes would have decreased the effect size that could be reliably detected
In this scenario, if the true effect size is δ ≤ 0.54, then there's less than a 50% chance that this design is likely to detect this effect size.
In other words, it's more likely to miss this effect than detect it
With a power contour plot in the methods section, readers (and reviewers) can quickly see what sort of effect sizes can be reliably detected and then make up their own mind whether these sort of effects are realistic and whether important effects sizes cannot be reliably detected
Sure, people can just write down the effect size that can be reliably detected with the study design. This is better than nothing, but people have different conceptions of what's considered "important".
Power contour plots show you a full range of effects, at a glance
• • •
Missing some Tweet in this thread? You can try to
force a refresh
And here's your annual reminder that a "study" cannot be underpowered, but rather, a *design and test combination* can be underpowered for detecting hypothetical effect sizes of interest towardsdatascience.com/why-you-should…
If you want to determine the range of hypothetical effect sizes a that given field can reliably detect (and reject), here's a demo with #Rstats code sciencedirect.com/science/articl…
My guide to calculating study-level statistical power for meta-analyses using the 'metameta' #Rstats package and web app is out now in AMPPS 🔓 doi.org/10.1177/251524…
Here's how this tool can be used for your next meta-analysis OR for re-analysing published meta-analyses 🧵
There's been a lot of talk recently about the quality of studies that are included in meta-analyses—how useful is a meta-analysis if it's just made up of studies with low evidential value?
But determining the evidential value of studies can be hard. Common approaches for looking at study quality or risk of bias tend to be quite subjective. You're not likely to get the same conclusions from different authors. These tasks can also be quite time consuming.
“Nearly 100% of published studies… confirm the initial hypothesis. This is an amazing accomplishment given the complexity of the human mind and human behaviour. Somehow, as psychological scientists, we always find the expected result; we always win!” royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rs…
One thing I want to add: the psychological sciences need to broadly adopt practices that help us determine when we're wrong. The standard NHST p-value approach cannot be used to provide support for absence of an effect. This paper provides two solutions academic.oup.com/psychsocgeront…
Strong auxiliary assumptions are also required for falsifying hypotheses
Participants who ate soup from bowls that were refillable, unbeknownst to them, ate 73% more soup than those eating from normal bowls pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15761167/
Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) expression patterns in the brain across development osf.io/j3b5d/
Here we identify OXTR gene expression patterns across the lifespan along with gene-gene co-expression patterns and potential health implications
[THREAD]
So, let's begin with some background.
As well as being an oxytocin researcher I'm also a meta-scientist, which means that a lot of my work on improving research methods is focused on improving oxytocin research (that's what got me into meta-science in the first place)
Earlier this year, we published a paper, led by @fuyu00, in which we proposed that three things are required to improving the precision of intranasal oxytocin research: Improved methods, reproducibility, and theory.