When engineers and architects tackle complex spatial problems, does all the action take place inside their heads? Not at all, write a team of learning scientists from Northwestern University. Such experts “don’t solve these problems just by manipulating mental models."
1/7 Image
"They solve them through the coordinated manipulation of both internal and external representations”—like sketches, models, and even hand gestures. But the instruction we offer students fails to develop the outside-the-brain thinking skills used by real-world experts.
2/7
“Traditional school mathematics privileges analytic approaches (i.e., formulas and calculations) over spatial ones (i.e., graphs, models, and diagrams)," the Northwestern researchers note in a recent paper.
3/7
Such traditional approaches also evaluate students’ skills "in ways that deprive them of the very tools (e.g., CAD software, coordinate grids) and collaborative structures (e.g., talking and gesturing through spatial ideas)" that would help them learn to think like experts. 4/7
Their solution: incorporate "making activities" into the teaching of math and science. By their very nature, such hands-on projects promote the use of the body, the use of materials in the physical environment, and the use of social supports for thinking and learning.
5/7
"Our analyses show that during making activities, students engaged in frequent and diverse spatial reasoning with a variety of social and material resources," the researchers write of their study, which involved 5th- and 6th-grade students.
6/7
Spatial experts like engineers and architects don't limit themselves to "brainbound" thinking. Why should we expect our students to do so?

Here's the paper, by Kay E. Ramey, Reed Stevens, and David H. Uttal: bit.ly/3rRDf2p
7/7

• • •

Missing some Tweet in this thread? You can try to force a refresh
 

Keep Current with Annie Murphy Paul

Annie Murphy Paul Profile picture

Stay in touch and get notified when new unrolls are available from this author!

Read all threads

This Thread may be Removed Anytime!

PDF

Twitter may remove this content at anytime! Save it as PDF for later use!

Try unrolling a thread yourself!

how to unroll video
  1. Follow @ThreadReaderApp to mention us!

  2. From a Twitter thread mention us with a keyword "unroll"
@threadreaderapp unroll

Practice here first or read more on our help page!

More from @anniemurphypaul

3 Apr
It's been a really weird year for college students. In thinking about the kind of supports they need, there's one we often overlook: their personal connection to the PLACE where they live.
1/6 Image
Benjamin Meagher, a psychologist at Kenyon College, recently published a paper in which he examined the relationship between college students' psychological well-being and the degree of "place identity" they experience in regard to their housing.
2/6
Place identity, he writes, "is characterized by the extent to which a setting reflects, reinforces, and communicates the identity of its resident"—in short, it's a feeling of connection with one’s physical setting.
3/6
Read 6 tweets
2 Apr
Andy Clark is one of the world's most interesting thinkers, and I'm always curious to hear what he's thinking about NOW. (With fellow philosopher David Chalmers, Clark proposed the theory of the extended mind, and is the author of "Supersizing the Mind," among other books.)
1/10 Image
Anthony Wing Kosner recently published an article about the extended mind, featuring an interview with Clark. It includes a number of gems—such as Clark's suggestion that the isolation imposed by the pandemic has driven us further into what he calls "brainbound" thinking.
2/10
"Intense digital home-working scenarios could feed a kind of isolationist user-illusion, enhancing our native tendencies to a kind of dualism" between the mind and its environment, Clark told Kosner.
3/10
Read 10 tweets
1 Apr
When engineers and architects tackle complex spatial problems, does all the action take place inside their heads? Not at all, write a team of learning scientists from Northwestern University. Such experts “don’t solve these problems just by manipulating mental models." 1/7 Image
"They solve them through the coordinated manipulation of both internal and external representations”—like sketches, models, and even hand gestures. But the instruction we offer students fails to develop the outside-the-brain thinking skills used by real-world experts. 2/7
“Traditional school mathematics privileges analytic approaches (i.e., formulas and calculations) over spatial ones (i.e., graphs, models, and diagrams)," the Northwestern researchers note in a recent paper. 3/7
Read 7 tweets
27 Mar
Communicating via videoconference makes us LESS intelligent, as a group, than we would be if we communicated by phone with just our voices. That’s the surprising finding of a new study by researchers at Carnegie Mellon University. The reason why is fascinating. 1/8
Research previously found that SYNCHRONY among group members—the alignment of non-verbal behaviors—promotes collective intelligence. You might think that synchrony would be easier to achieve when members can see each other—but in fact such visual cues act as distractions. 2/8
Groups that communicated using voice only paid close attention to auditory cues and achieved higher levels of synchrony. They were also more equal in their turn-taking—another factor that promotes collective intelligence. 3/8
Read 8 tweets
26 Mar
I've been reading a fascinating new book that is all about how the SPACES in which teachers and students operate affect the learning that takes place there. It includes one of my favorite anecdotes about the role of physical space in our thinking processes. 1/8
After the British House of Commons was severely damaged by German bombs in 1941, Winston Churchill weighed in on plans for the reconstruction of the building, writes the book's co-editor, Thomas Kvan, in an introductory essay. 2/8
Churchill believed that the reconstructed room should retain its rectangular shape, arguing that "the narrow rectilinear form of the chamber forced debaters to take clear positions, unlike a semi-circular space that facilitated subtle nuances by displacements along an arc." 3/8
Read 8 tweets
25 Mar
Designers of digital tools aim to make them as “seamless” as possible—such that our technology supplies us with the information we need right away, without us having to ask for it. But it’s precisely these qualities that undermine our own sense of how difficult a task is. 1/8 Image
An interesting new paper by Matthew Fisher and Daniel Oppenheimer in Psychological Science looks at what happens when our mental work is augmented by technological resources—say, using spellcheck to correct our writing. 2/8
The use of such "external sources," they found, appears to "distort metacognitive assessments of one’s own abilities." In other words, when we extend our minds with technology, we tend to lose touch with how hard the task is and what we would be able to do on our own. 3/8
Read 8 tweets

Did Thread Reader help you today?

Support us! We are indie developers!


This site is made by just two indie developers on a laptop doing marketing, support and development! Read more about the story.

Become a Premium Member ($3/month or $30/year) and get exclusive features!

Become Premium

Too expensive? Make a small donation by buying us coffee ($5) or help with server cost ($10)

Donate via Paypal Become our Patreon

Thank you for your support!

Follow Us on Twitter!