Crémieux Profile picture
I write about genetics, 'metrics, and demographics. Read my long-form writing at https://t.co/8hgA4nNS2A.
49 subscribers
Oct 7 10 tweets 4 min read
What predicts a successful educational intervention?

Unfortunately, the answer is not 'methodological propriety'; in fact, it's the opposite🧵

First up: home-made measures, a lack of randomization, and a study being published instead of unpublished predict larger effects. Image It is *far* easier to cook the books with an in-house measure, and it's far harder for other researchers to evaluate what's going on because they definitionally cannot be familiar with it.

Additionally, smaller studies tend to have larger effects—a hallmark of publication bias! Image
Oct 6 6 tweets 3 min read
Across five different large samples, the same pattern emerged:

Trans people tended to have multiple times higher rates of autism. Image In addition to higher autism rates, when looking at non-autistic trans versus non-trans people, the trans people were consistently shifted towards showing more autistic traits. Image
Oct 6 6 tweets 3 min read
Across 68,000 meta-analyses including over 700,000 effect size estimates, correcting for publication bias tended to:

- Markedly reduce effect sizes
- Markedly reduce the probability that there is an effect at all

Economics hardest hit: Image Even this is perhaps too generous.

Recall that correcting for publication bias often produces effects that are still larger than the effects attained in subsequent large-scale replication studies.Image
Oct 5 4 tweets 2 min read
Neat new article from @Scientific_Bird.

It argues that one of the reasons there was an East Asian growth miracle but not a South Asian one is human capital.

For centuries, South Asia has lagged on average human capital, whereas East Asia has done very well in all our records. Image It's unsurprising when these things continue today.

We already know based on three separate instrumental variables strategies using quite old datapoints that human capital is causal for growth. That includes these numeracy measures from the distant past.

Image
Oct 4 13 tweets 5 min read
The results are in and 58.3% of the almost 7,500 responses said...

Men tend to get more steps in a day!

Sources say...

Yes. Wherever we have large-scale, representatively sampled data, men tend to get a few more steps in compared to women. Image Step counts tend to vary by area.

For example, New York—thanks to New York City—has the highest average step count.

Colorado—due in part to its selected active, athletic population—also manages a high step count.

You'll also notice that moderate temps mean more steps. Image
Oct 1 10 tweets 5 min read
Stats on the homeless population are abysmal.

One-in-two has a disability and/or a traumatic brain injury. One-in-five has psychosis. One-in-ten is schizophrenic. One-in-four is mentally retarded.

These facts have major consequences! Image As I noted recently, the White House wants to bring back involuntary commitment.

They're probably in the right to call for that, since so many homeless are incapable of taking care of themselves, or at the very least, not hurting others.

Image
Sep 29 6 tweets 2 min read
I really don't get the Mormon hate.

They are genuinely the nicest people I've ever met and a joy to be around.

You can insult their religion, call it weird, say it's made up, and they just tell you that everyone's entitled to an opinion and ask if you want to play board games. Every reddit atheist interaction with them is like

"You still believe in Joseph Smith even though he was a criminal?"

"Yeah! By the way what do you think of the ice cream? It's homemade. Picked the strawberries myself!"
Sep 27 4 tweets 1 min read
These are really high figures across the board!

What makes them worse is that plenty of moderates and large numbers of people on the left think random non-Nazis are Nazis, and thus acceptable to punch. Image With a sizable minority who think political violence is acceptable generally and a much larger number who think it's acceptable conditional on the right target, it feels crucial to start castigating people who use slurs like "racist" or "Nazi".

It's effectively incitement.
Sep 26 5 tweets 2 min read
We know the answer to this question already.

The AAP gave out bad advice: they told parents to avoid giving their kids peanuts.

But as the LEAP trial showed, parents giving their kids peanuts early in life reduces the rates of peanut allergy by about 70%. Image Israel has the solution: Bamba!

Stop avoiding peanuts and there won't be much of a peanut allergy issue to speak of.

It's that simple. Bad advice to parents created a generation of people with an unnecessary allergy. Image
Sep 26 4 tweets 2 min read
Details are scarce, but it appears Trump is about to double the price of...

80% of drugs?!

We have no idea if this applies to Bulk, APIs, or just finished drugs. It only says "Pharmaceutical Product", but just in case, I have simple advice: stock up now!Image Details:

Of the top 100 brand-name drugs by Part D spend, 67 are finished outside the U.S.: pharmacychecker.com/research/not-m…

FDA holds the U.S. did 28% of API manufacturing in 2019: fda.gov/news-events/co…

GAO, citing FDA, says 40% of finished drugs, 80% of API: gao.gov/assets/gao-20-…
Sep 25 9 tweets 4 min read
About 78% of those arrested by ICE in Republican states are either criminals, have charges pending, or have committed some other violation.

In Democratic states, the number is about 60%. Image Another interesting thing is that, as the arrests have increased, the severity has fallen:

The people ICE is arresting aren't as seriously criminal as they used to be, but there are more of them.

It's interesting to see this common tradeoff crop up in deportations, too! Image
Sep 23 9 tweets 4 min read
This replicates in many places.

For example, in Denmark, the broader the definition of autism (blue = broadest; red = narrowest), the more autism diagnoses have increased.

Crucially, this study also replicated the finding that symptoms are stable, while diagnoses are up.Image I've previously noted that this same thing was observed elsewhere.

For example, it was seen in Sweden: stable symptom scores (i.e., the things defining autism), but people kept getting diagnosed at higher rates. Image
Sep 23 14 tweets 5 min read
On the left, you can see child autism diagnoses.

On the right, you can see states with policies that give schools more money when their students are diagnosed with autism.

When these policies pass, autism diagnoses increase by almost 25% in one year! Image Incentives matter for autism diagnosis.

For example, people on SSI receive larger payouts if they're diagnosed with autism.

After the economic downturn in 2008, the most heavily impacted age group started getting diagnosed with autism at an incredible rate: Image
Sep 22 20 tweets 7 min read
The thing about anti-vaxxers is that they don't know things

They clamor to find ways to suggest vaccines are bad, but their arguments are silly because they don't know the basic institutional background that gave rise to today's "autism epidemic"

Thread on a ridiculous paper🧵 Image This paper is by David A. Geier.

He's had some papers retracted.

A lot of his work has to do with other people having conflicts of interest—like working at a public health agency—, which makes some of the retractions extra funny, because they've been about his COIs. Image
Sep 15 13 tweets 11 min read
I am extremely grateful for the publication of this result.

Why? Because it means this method has gone mainstream and replications can flourish by using it.

So, here's the first replication, all using the U.K. Biobank. This is for Black-White differences in IQ: Image Notes!

0. The sample for the population result is *everyone* with ≥1/16 and ≤15/16 African admixture, the within-family result is for the siblings among them. I didn't plot between-families results, but they're pulled from the same distribution as the population result, so when I have those computed I'll replot, but they shouldn't be any different (will take a few hours, maybe be tomorrow, w/e).

1. Yes, datasets will be merged and sharper results will be obtained for the article on this that's coming out ~shortly. Can't be done for all phenotypes in the UKBB, but can be done for IQ at least. For example, we don't have lipoprotein(a) (lp(a)) measurements in some of the samples of young American kids, some of them don't have objective skin color measurements, etc. As a side note, this result holds up with brain size (not shown) in the UKBB, but I'm unsure if it holds up in the young samples that'll be merged with it, as they're still developing in most cases.

Anyway, the IQ p-value is p = 0.008 (two-tailed) within families and extremely low between them. The within-family result is robust, will get sharper with more data, and is also sharpened by using a latent variable instead of a score, and by correcting for measurement error (not needed with the LVM). Score shown for simplicity and ease of others with UKBB access replicating this. Also, accounting for error in the admixture computation, the p-value would drop a bit further, but not by very much since error is very small.

2. The "IQ per unit of admixture" is statistically indistinguishable between the population and within-family results, and yes, it explains most of the Black-White difference in IQ. I just wanted comparably-scaled results for all the traits here, so you're seeing r's. It's pleasant that the within-family variance reductions aren't enormous for siblings, which is what we expect even with quite high heritabilities given their genetic relatedness. It's the same result we've seen with American data, and it's also nice to see that in the case of this trait, the global admixture result *can* be interpreted like the within-family one. Presumably this only holds with measurement invariance, as we see in the U.K. when comparing Whites and Blacks there. Since we see this in the U.S. too, it's likely that the previous, already-published within-family null—which had a sizable effect in the correct direction which also could not be distinguished from the global r—was just a false-negative.

3. This result replicates with other degrees of relatedness, but we might lose the causal interpretation with those ones because the estimand for, say, a cousin test is different given the identity of the "C" variance component shifts for that comparison.

4. What is architectural sparsity and why is it relevant? Consider this table from nature.com/articles/s4159… (cc: @hsu_steve):

Basically, sparsity refers to the number of variants involved in a trait. It also refers to their effect size distribution. So, for Alzheimer's, for example, the trait is highly polygenic, but APOE explains more variance than the entire rest of the PGS, so while being under highly polygenic control, it remains moderately sparse.

If you're still not grokking what I mean here, consider some distributions of cumulative effects across the chromosomes. Here's the result for lp(a), which is already known to be influenced by essentially one gene. Guess which chromosome it's located on:

Consider, as a comparison, creatinine, which is considerably less sparse, and thus has effects distributed across all chromosomes:

Now, consider what this does to within-family admixture assessments. Skin color, for example, is controlled by only a handful of genes. This means that global admixture shouldn't tag it very much between siblings. The same is even more true for lp(a). And in fact, now we have confirmation of this!

But, we know from other methods that lp(a) is effectively entirely genetically-caused between populations. This is just accepted within the medical community because it is an obvious fact that follows from its strong control by a single gene. You can also figure this out using local ancestry estimates. Basically, the correlation between genome-wide ancestry and ancestry at a causal loci is what we want to get at, and if you know the causal loci, you gain power by restricting your analysis to that area. This is what we find with lp(a) (not shown, but use your brain. The obviousness of this fact is why the author used lp(a) as an example).

Also, in some sense, the frequency of that locus between ancestries gives you what you need without doing all this within-family stuff, if you're confident it's causal. The effect estimate might still be biased by population structure though, so that's worth keeping in mind.

There will be a post soon with more details and the expanded set of results with the additional datasets, robustness tests, and plenty of other fun things to look at.

TL;DR: this is a spoiler, and it shows that, yes, you can explain the Black-White IQ difference in Britain mostly genetically, and the global admixture result that I've posted here before is equivalent to the within-family one. Woohoo for things that should hold up, in fact, holding up!

As a sort of replication of the Young paper, you cannot explain the difference in educational attainment (as years of education) in this way. Why? Well, hard to say. Compensatory factors like I found with the GCSEs? (Haven't read that post yet? Go check it out here: cremieux.xyz/p/explaining-a…). Poor phenotype quality? Very plausible, because education really is a huge garbage heap, but why would that be in the general population and not within families? Maybe it has to do with what other traits admixture tags? Maybe it does replicate, but we just can't see it, because the precision is too poor (possibly the lp(a) story too). Who knows!

Q: Will the combination with more datasets allow us to fix this educational attainment result?

A: No, because most of the other datasets involve young people, not people who have almost all completed their educations, as in the UKBB. That plus the generational change and international incomparability in the definition of educational attainment makes it too poor as a phenotype. Sorry!

Any questions?

Link to a fun previous post from the same dataset, showing a result that *does* hold up within families: x.com/cremieuxrecuei…

Link to another post mentioning the forthcoming article earlier: x.com/cremieuxrecuei…Image
Image
Image
Sep 14 8 tweets 3 min read
Ever wondered why advertisements heavily feature Black actors when they're just 12-14% of the population?

I might have an explanation:

Black viewers have a strong preference for seeing other Blacks in media, whereas Whites have no racial preferences. Image These results are derived from a meta-analysis of 57 pre-2000 and 112 post-2000 effect sizes for Blacks alongside 76 and 87 such effect sizes for Whites.

If you look at them, you'll notice that Whites' initial, slight preference declined and maybe reversed. Image
Sep 14 10 tweets 3 min read
You're on trial, and the jury can't make up their minds. The decision is a coin flip: 50/50, you either get it or you don't.

Your odds of a given verdict depend on the "peers" making up your jury.

If you're Black and they're Black, your odds are good; if you're White, pray. Image Though White jurors have, on average, no racial bias, the same can't be said for Black jurors.

Where the White jury gives you approximately the coin flip you deserve, the Black jury's odds for a verdict are like a coin rigged to come up heads 62% of the time.
Sep 14 4 tweets 1 min read
Posts saying Charlie said things he didn't, oftentimes even including videos where he doesn't say what the post says he does, have convinced me.

There will be no organic temperature-lowering coming from the left, because they don't want it.

They believe the things they claim. They actually believe the people they dislike *are* racists, fascists, homophobes, transphobes, all of it.

They are not reasonable, and they cannot form anything like a reasoned argument for these perceptions.

Even still, it's really what they think.
Sep 13 12 tweets 2 min read
Your child's teacher says that people like you deserve to be killed for your beliefs.

They also laughed at the fact that when people like you are killed, their kids lose a parent, because again, 'people like you deserve it'.

You believe the teacher should be: An airline pilot said that people like you should be killed for your beliefs.

You believe the pilot should be:
Sep 13 12 tweets 4 min read
A few days ago, I posted this image.

It shows that the gender wage gap is mostly about married men and their exceptional earnings.

In this thread, I'm going to explain why married men earn so much more than everyone else🧵Image The question is:

Does marriage maketh man?

Or

Are all the good men married?

That is, does marriage lead men to earn more, or do men who earn more get married more often? Image
Sep 12 5 tweets 10 min read
I've seen a lot of people wondering why America has such a high incarceration rate.

If you weren't even aware that it does, consider this graph from Prison Policy:

To understand why America is like this, consider that, when Stalin died, his secret police chief Lavrentiy Beria released more than a million non-political prisoners and the result was a massive crime wave.

This is not the only instance of this happening in history. Plenty of places have done large-scale prisoner releases, and they nearly universally have the same effects wherever they happen: crime goes up.

One of my favorite examples comes from Italy.

On July 31st, 2006, the Italian Parliament passed the Collective Clemency Bill. This bill reduced the sentences of eligible inmates convicted prior to May 2nd, 2006 by three years, effective August 1st, 2006. As a result, thousands of inmates were released immediately. In fact, 83% of all releases through December, 2007, happened in August, 2006.

The pardon was motivated by the activism of the Catholic Church, including personal involvement from Pope John Paul II. The Catholics argued that prisons were overfilled, holding people in crowded conditions was inhumane, and a release was needed. They also had historical precedent on their side: after the second World War, there were regular collective pardons in Italy, but they stopped in 1992 after a parliamentary change, making the 2006 pardon the first of its kind in fourteen odd years.

Researchers Buonanno & Raphael documented what happened when the pardon went into effect. First, take a look at the incarceration rates over time:

Prior to the pardon, incarceration rates were trending up fairly slowly.

Afterwards, they trended up at a much more rapid rate!

In fact, the incarceration rate converged back to roughly where the whole thing started after less than three years. By December 2008, it had reached a rate of 98 again, compared to 103 in August of 2006.

The reason why the incarceration rate rapidly returned to the level it was initially at isn't terribly shocking: it's because crime increased!

In response to a major increase in crime, police had to start arresting more people. Recidivists and those otherwise driven to crime by the release of so many criminals needed to be arrested or the crime spree would have carried on.

In other words, incarceration incapacitates criminals, and when you shock the incarceration rate by releasing tons of criminals from a state of being incapacitated, crime goes up until they're put back in jail.

Well, unless you're fine living with a higher crime rate. If you are, then the incarceration rate can remain at a lower level.

There's a tradeoff here: if country A has a population that tends to commit few crimes regardless of policy, they can have low incarceration rates. But if country B has a population that tends to commit many crimes regardless of policy, they'll have to settle for having higher incapacitation rates if they want to realize crime levels like country A.

The populations differ in terms of antecedents of crime, so the treatment of those populations has to differ if they're going to achieve the same results.

This clears up why America has such a high incarceration rate: it's because Americans are relatively violent people!

This also tells us why El Salvador's efforts have been such a success. But before being explicit about that, here's another result from Buonanno & Raphael.

Leveraging cross-province differences in the numbers of people pardoned, they found that incapacitation effects on crime were larger when the province had a lower pre-pardon incarceration rate! Or in other words, there were diminishing returns to increased incarceration!

The reason for this is that the population is constantly in flux. There's growth, there's immigration and emigration, there's death—people come and go. There'll always be someone who is going to commit another crime. If we're lucky, there'll also always be someone there to catch them.

Some people commit more crimes than others. If you lock up all of the worst offenders, you can seriously reduce crime. For example,

- In Sweden, 1958-1980, a rigorously enforced three-strike law could have halved violent crime (x.com/cremieuxrecuei…). In this example, it was found that 1% of the Swedish population did 63% of their violent crimes.
- In America, the vast majority of people admitted to state prisons, 2009-2014, were repeat offenders (x.com/cremieuxrecuei…)
- In cities like Chicago, Atlanta, D.C., Portland, and basically everywhere else, homicide victims and offenders tend to have long rap sheets (cremieux.xyz/p/minority-rep…)

These are fairly universal findings! Crime is very concentrated: within regions, within cities, along streets, among a few people, within a few ages. The further down you go, the greater the concentration of crime perpetration in general.

The reason higher pre-pardon incarceration rates meant smaller incapacitation effects was because the worst offenders tended to be locked up already in those areas. Accordingly, if you lock up the marginal offender in a high incarceration area, you prevent fewer crimes from happening compared to if you lock up Vincenzo Megamurderer who has a rap sheet longer than a foot race.

And this replicates!

- Vollaard found that a 2001 law passed in the Netherlands that handed down ten times longer sentences to prolific offenders reduced rates of theft by 25%. This was subject to diminishing returns: as municipalities dipped deeper into the pool of repeat offenders in applying repeat offender sentence enhancements, the incapacitation effect got smaller.
- Johnson & Raphael found that between 1978 and 1990 in the U.S., each additional prison year served prevented 14 serious crimes. At the time, the average incarceration rate was 186 per 100,000. In the period 1991 through 2004, each additional prison year served prevented was just 3, and 2.6 of those being property crimes. In this period, the average incarceration rate was 396 per 100,000. America had hit the point of diminishing returns.

In elasticity terms, the Italian collective pardon revealed a crime-prison elasticity of -0.4, and with dynamic adjustment, they were as high as -0.66.

- Johnson & Raphael found crime-prison elasticities of -0.43 for property crime and -0.79 for violent crime for the 1978-1990 period.
- Levitt used prison overcrowding litigation as an instrument to estimate the crime-prison elasticity with data from the late-1970s through to the early-1990s, and he found elasticities of -0.38 to -0.42 for violent and -0.26 to -0.32 for property crime.
- A year after Buonanno & Raphael's study, Barbarino & Mastrobuoni published their own analysis of Italian collective pardons for the eight pardons laid out in the period 1962-1990. They found an elasticity of total crime ranging between -0.17 and -0.30.
- Buonanno et al. found that, in a comparison of the U.S. and Europe, the crime-prison elasticity was -0.40. They were able to do this estimation because, modern Europe at the time had developed higher property and violent crime rates than the U.S. (excluding homicide), so they exploited panel data on the reversal of misfortunes that implied.

So, back to El Salvador: they currently have the lowest homicide rates in the western hemisphere.

Some people claim they've been on this path since 2015, but it's hard to make this case, when their reversion from that year's peak was consistent with regression to the mean, and regression to the mean does not tend to make things better than ever before. It was very likely the massive lockup of people who were confirmed criminals that has brought El Salvador this level of unprecedented peace.

To put a pin in this: incarceration rates are endogenous!

Different places have different incarceration rates because they have different underlying rates of crime, different levels of and population support for and cooperation with policing, and different tolerances for keeping criminals locked up or set free. Places are in different equilibriums for numerous reasons, which is why comparisons of incarceration, policing, and crime rates are often facially meaningless. It simply makes no sense to make an unqualified statement like 'Incarceration doesn't work - just look at Louisiana, which has both high incarceration and high crime!'

To really understand the linkage between incarceration and crime requires causally informative research like the wonderful work I cited on Italy's collective pardons. To really grasp the thorny issue of crime in general requires plying your counterfactual reasoning skills so that you don't make a silly mistake like saying:

Alaskans wear bigger, puffier, more insulating coats than Floridians, yet they suffer more hypothermia deaths. Therefore, we can reject that view that coats help people to stay warm.

Sources: cremieux.xyz/p/a-twitterx-d…

Bonus! An earlier thread on counterfactual reasoning about a different topic: x.com/cremieuxrecuei…Image
Image
Image
Image
Bonus post:

You can plausibly trade off policing and incarceration. If you choose to police more, you can incarcerate less with the same crime rate and vice-versa.