Cochlear implant fact of the day: there is no credible evidence that sign language causes poor spoken language outcomes in children with CIs. Read this one carefully! Image description: pale blue background with circles and sca
Little research exists on early sign language access in children with CIs specifically.
Some research has reported better spoken language outcomes for DHH children who communicate primarily in spoken language compared to those who communicate primarily in sign language. In some cases, study authors have suggested that sign language itself causes poorer outcomes.
A systematic review of the research on early sign and spoken language access for DHH children was published in 2016. The authors reported that not enough high-quality research exists on this topic.
Whether sign language + spoken language or spoken language interventions alone are more effective cannot be determined from the available evidence (Fitzpatrick et al., 2016).
Some of the available research on this topic is what scientists call "correlational": it suggests that two things are related but does not tell us *how*. It does not show cause and effect, and there are other possible explanations for the research findings.
For example: quality of spoken language access may influence a child’s communication preferences at specific points in time and long term. A child who has less spoken language access with hearing devices may prefer sign language because they find it more accessible.
Degree of spoken language access itself is a possible explanation for a child’s spoken language outcome.
Geers et al. (2017) is a commonly cited example of evidence for focusing on spoken language rather than sign language. The problem with this is that the evidence it provides is not strong enough to conclude that sign language causes poorer spoken language outcomes.
If you read this article, read it carefully and be sure to read the responses to it as well. Several researchers in linguistics, psychology, and related fields have written thoughtful responses (linked below) challenging the conclusions of this research.
Another issue to consider: studies on this topic do not define “sign language” in a consistent way. American Sign Language is independent of English. It has its own grammar and is different from English-based sign communication systems (like sign-supported speech).
In some studies, all forms of signing are lumped into "sign language" or the term “sign language” is not defined at all. When this happens, there is no way to know how many children had access to a sign language vs. an English-based signing system.
Further, most research on this topic does not control for variations in children’s language experiences. A child who “prefers sign language” might be one who has had sign language access for their whole life, one who wasn't exposed to a sign language until later, ...
...or many other circumstances. Variations in language experience introduce different possible explanations for outcomes. Other research (cited below) suggests that sign language access may support spoken language development.
The relationship of spoken language outcomes to sign language access in children with CIs is not yet understood fully. We should not jump to the conclusion that sign language causes poor spoken language outcomes.
For citations and links, read below.

Systematic review:
-Fitzpatrick, E. M., Hamel, C., Stevens, A., Pratt, M., Moher, D., Doucet, S. P., ... & Na, E. (2016). Sign language and spoken language for children with hearing loss: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 137(1), e20151974.
Examples of research reporting better spoken language outcomes for DHH children who communicate primarily in spoken language:
-Boons, T., Brokx, J. P., Dhooge, I., Frijns, J. H., Peeraer, L., Vermeulen, A., ... & Van Wieringen, A. (2012). Predictors of spoken language development following pediatric cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 33(5), 617-639.
-Geers, A. E., Mitchell, C. M., Warner-Czyz, A., Wang, N. Y., Eisenberg, L. S., & CDaCI Investigative Team. (2017). Early sign language exposure and cochlear implantation benefits. Pediatrics, 140(1).
Examples of research suggesting that sign language access may support spoken language development:
-Davidson, K., Lillo-Martin, D., & Chen Pichler, D. (2014). Spoken English language development among native signing children with cochlear implants. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(2), 238-250.
-Petitto, L. A., Katerelos, M., Levy, B. G., Gauna, K., Tétreault, K., & Ferraro, V. (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 453.
Article-length critique of Geers et al. (2017):
-Hall, M. L., Hall, W. C., & Caselli, N. K. (2019). Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Language, 39(4), 367-395.
Comments on Geers et al. (2017), including critiques
pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/140/1/…

This list is not comprehensive. Feel free to add more resources and citations to this thread!

• • •

Missing some Tweet in this thread? You can try to force a refresh
 

Keep Current with Audiology Outside the Box PLLC

Audiology Outside the Box PLLC Profile picture

Stay in touch and get notified when new unrolls are available from this author!

Read all threads

This Thread may be Removed Anytime!

PDF

Twitter may remove this content at anytime! Save it as PDF for later use!

Try unrolling a thread yourself!

how to unroll video
  1. Follow @ThreadReaderApp to mention us!

  2. From a Twitter thread mention us with a keyword "unroll"
@threadreaderapp unroll

Practice here first or read more on our help page!

More from @AudOTB

13 Jan
Listening fatigue is a common experience for deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) people who communicate in spoken language. Understanding what another person has said requires cognitive effort. Image description: dark blue Monserrat Alternates Bold text
Even more of that effort is required when a person does not hear all of the sounds clearly due to background noise or another reason. When a person is experiencing listening fatigue, they may feel tired, frustrated, and stressed, even after the listening situation is over.
They will probably have more difficulty maintaining attention and energy. This picture lists other examples too. Another person's listening fatigue might not be obvious. You might not always know that the DHH person in your life is experiencing listening fatigue.
Read 7 tweets
12 Jan
Today's cochlear implant myths and facts post has been challenging to write. An important distinction must be made clearly. People with different perspectives might say that "CIs work!" or "CIs don't work!" Which is correct? The answer depends upon how you define "work". Image description: pale blue background with circles and sca
CIs provide access to sound. They are highly effective for this, and almost every person who uses a CI hears more than they would with a hearing aid.
If you are a CI candidate and your expectation is better hearing than you would have with a hearing aid, that is realistic in most cases. But if your expectation is "normal" hearing and no difficulties with listening, that is not realistic.
Read 10 tweets
11 Jan
Cochlear implant fact of the day: those inspirational CI activation videos that you see on the internet do not tell the whole story. Image description: pale blue background with circles and sca
When you see those videos, you see only the reaction from the CI user and/or their parents. What you don't see is how the CI user is hearing in that moment and all of the work that lies ahead of them in learning how to hear through their CI.
You might be wondering, "What's wrong with celebrating when a person has a good CI outcome?" Nothing at all! But whether an outcome is "good" isn't apparent from a CI activation video. Activation day is only the beginning of a long process.
Read 6 tweets
16 Nov 20
Dr. Sarah Sparks, Au.D. is pleased to announced the formation of Audiology Outside the Box, an online practice serving clients in Maryland, Massachusetts, Virginia, and Washington, DC. Practice and website opening on November 30, 2020!
Audiology Outside the Box offers a different mindset about audiology services. The focus is on supporting your communication and hearing-related goals, whether those involve spoken language or not. All services can be provided in American Sign Language (ASL), English, or both.
What happens during your appointment? You share about your needs and preferences, and your audiologist will listen. Together, we will create a plan that suits your individuality and does not force you into a box.
Read 7 tweets

Did Thread Reader help you today?

Support us! We are indie developers!


This site is made by just two indie developers on a laptop doing marketing, support and development! Read more about the story.

Become a Premium Member ($3/month or $30/year) and get exclusive features!

Become Premium

Too expensive? Make a small donation by buying us coffee ($5) or help with server cost ($10)

Donate via Paypal Become our Patreon

Thank you for your support!

Follow Us on Twitter!