Whole word reading instruction was rooted in Cattell’s (1886) research. He carried out a series of laboratory studies at Wundt University in Germany utilising tachistoscopic techniques which measured eye fixation times on letters and words (Rayner et al., 2012).
Cattell (1886) discovered that in ten milliseconds a reader could apprehend equally well three or four unrelated letters, two unrelated words, or a short sentence of four words - approximately twenty-four letters (Anderson and Dearborn, 1952).
The generalisation advanced from this outcome was that words and sentences are easier to read than letters. This resulted in the deduction that humans do not read words by the serial decoding of individual letters but read whole words in their entirety.
However, he carried out his experiments on fluent readers with highly developed orthographic processing. In other words, they could already read.Thus, nearly all instruction in the 20thC was dominated by the precept that as words are read as wholes they must be taught as wholes.
Gates' (1927) research - despite a laughably tiny sample size (25) and heavily biased language, and being the only research study in the whole of the 20thC where participants outperform pupils taught using a code-based approach (Chall, 1965) concluded:
‘That it will be the part of wisdom to curtail the use of phonics instruction in the first grade very greatly, is strongly implied; indeed, it is not improbable that it should be eliminated entirely…’ (1927:226).It wouldn't have mattered...
...except that his position at the University of Columbia endowed him with significant authority, both in terms of research profile and instruction of trainee teachers, and his concepts gained impetus over the century – he was inducted into The Reading Hall of Fame in 1978.
Perhaps a salutary lesson in the necessity of viewing cumulative research. As Chall (1967) concluded:
‘Most children…are taught to read by…a meaning emphasis method. Yet the research from 1912 to 1965 indicates that a code-emphasis method -one that views beginning reading as essentially different from mature reading – produces better results…’ (1967: 307).

• • •

Missing some Tweet in this thread? You can try to force a refresh
 

Keep Current with The Reading Ape

The Reading Ape Profile picture

Stay in touch and get notified when new unrolls are available from this author!

Read all threads

This Thread may be Removed Anytime!

PDF

Twitter may remove this content at anytime! Save it as PDF for later use!

Try unrolling a thread yourself!

how to unroll video
  1. Follow @ThreadReaderApp to mention us!

  2. From a Twitter thread mention us with a keyword "unroll"
@threadreaderapp unroll

Practice here first or read more on our help page!

More from @TheReadingApe

31 Mar
Fluency is a continuum and not a threshold. It develops after orthographic skills (automatic word recognition) become embedded - the bottleneck in reading development. This appears to be self-taught (Share, 2004) so requires heavy reading mileage and presentation of words.
Don’t rush through this in a race to fluency and comprehension. Instant word recognition is a key development phase and is predicated on significant code knowledge. Hence the vital contribution from decodable texts.
Instant word recognition is arguably more important in primary education than fluency (which will never develop without it). It can take some time, be laborious and children often sound slow and stuttering whilst it develops but it is crucial.
Read 4 tweets
6 Mar
The 'language surplus' gifted by privilege often conflates early reading instruction and primary education in general as we try to repay a language/privilege deficit with undue haste in an understandable desire for social justice. Cognitive load will always apply.
Far better to ensure foundations are secure - particularly focusing on automaticity (arguably the main concern of primary education) in all areas, and in reading build to fluency slowly and surely. Discrete, global, cultural knowledge is assigned to the curriculum.
The deficit is unlikely to be repaid by the end of primary education but with fluency in place and the associated release of cognitive load, the opportunity for repayment of the deficit and building of substantial surplus and privilege is possible throughout secondary education.
Read 4 tweets
5 Mar
Sorry Mat, only just got to this.Beck and McKeown carried out considerable research on QtA and the studies on teacher activity and use of querying suggested that practices indeed developed. However, in terms of pupil outcomes their research was inconclusive.
Their 1996 study (as I said, this is old stuff) used a control group that received instruction from a basal programme. There were significant differences in favour of QtA between pupils who received QtA instruction and those who read without instructional support.
However, there were no significant differences between pupils who received QtA instruction and the basal instruction control group. These findings were repeated by Garcia et al. (2007) using 'responsive engagement' - similar to QtA. The control group had vocabulary instruction.
Read 8 tweets
3 Mar
Try these: Beck’s (1998) assertion that phoneme to grapheme mapping is the equivalent to reading as dribbling skills are to basketball: necessary but not sufficient to play the game. The implication is clear: without sufficient phonic knowledge, reading may not be possible.
Daniels and Diack (1956) - to ignore the alphabet when teaching the decoding of English is inexplicable.
Whole word method is undermined by the capacity of humans to remember a limited number of symbols. Chinese children are expected to recognise only 3500 characters by the age eleven (Leong, 1973), and it takes twelve years of study to learn 2000 logographs in Japanese(Gough,2006).
Read 9 tweets
3 Mar
Reciprocal Reading treats reading comprehension as a skill. As such it is in denial of Kahn's (2007) assertion that it is not a skill, but a complex mix of thinking, reasoning, imagining and interpreting and is a largely knowledge-based process dependent on content knowledge
It assigns roles to pupils (predictor, clarifier, questioner, summariser) in a small group who 'collaborate'. It seems to have evolved from Collaborative Strategic Reading (Kim, 2006).The technique is used in Success For All - much criticised by Kozol (2006) and McGuinness (1999)
The research that implied it was successful was undermined by the evidence that pretty much any comprehension skills teaching was effective. Reading seemed to improve reading. More here on skills-based comprehension strategies: thereadingape.com/single-post/20…
Read 8 tweets
2 Mar
Now there's a good question! There is no source because there is no recognised model for effective reading instruction in the UK which as @EdinspireGeoff states has 4 separate national bodies.
The only universal expectation is phonics teaching in Reception and Year 1 and once the phonics screening check threshold has been crossed this may stop - despite comprehensive code knowledge not yet having been achieved.
Beyond this pretty much any approach goes from whole language to reading dogs. Guided reading still maintains traction along with numerous schemes that mainly focus on comprehension 'skills'. These include the 'power of reading' which includes precious little reading...
Read 9 tweets

Did Thread Reader help you today?

Support us! We are indie developers!


This site is made by just two indie developers on a laptop doing marketing, support and development! Read more about the story.

Become a Premium Member ($3/month or $30/year) and get exclusive features!

Become Premium

Too expensive? Make a small donation by buying us coffee ($5) or help with server cost ($10)

Donate via Paypal Become our Patreon

Thank you for your support!

Follow Us on Twitter!