Thanks to @ryandal for putting me onto this brilliant paper by @GerardSeijts et al. This absolutely nails why teacher performance goals aren't helpful if teachers don't *already* have all the skills/resources to achieve the performance. citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/downlo…
A performance goal such as 'achieve these class results' is only helpful for a teacher if they already have all the perceptive understanding of students' challenges, the skill and resources to meet them. It encourages them to prioritise attention/time/effort to this goal.
But if 'doing more of the same' or redoubling of existing efforts won't cut it, then use of a performance goal instead of a learning goal is likely to hinder the achievement of better results - it makes teachers less likely to focus on problem-solving & learning.
Importantly, the learning goals stimulate increased self-efficacy which correlates with greater likelihood of 'information search'
There's some more nuanced findings from another paper (again HT @ryandal) by Masuda (@EADABusiness) et al where they find that a combination of learning *and* performance goals can also be effective. researchgate.net/publication/28…
Interestingly, Masuda et al also found that, initially, increasing task challenge was positively associated with performance, but this plateaued and then ultimately fell off. They hypothesise a link with working memory limits.
What would this mean for school performance management? Firstly, recognising that performance goals alone may only be suitable when sufficient understanding, skill, perceptive expertise, self-efficacy, resources are all in place.
Secondly, it's important to consider separately proximal v distal goals - e.g. 'how my class does in this current topic' v 'how my class does at the end of GCSE' and how different combinations may be better for novices v experienced teachers.
Once again these papers fundamentally reinforce the idea that your performance management system *must* have a strong link to ongoing professional learning if you don't want to actively reduce teachers' performance.
• • •
Missing some Tweet in this thread? You can try to
force a refresh
This is a great paper by Joseph E. Mroz and colleagues on the science of workplace meetings. (short thread 🧵) - this area is absolutely core to @TeacherDevTrust's view on effective school leadership.
They begin by summarising some key findings from three key areas of meeting science.
And then go on with a helpful check-list of factors that promote good meetings.
A thread with some advice on videoconferencing - I'm focusing mainly on using Zoom. Firstly, why Zoom? Many of the safety worries have been resolved and the stability, resilience and breakout rooms features are great. (More on safety: tidbits.com/2020/04/03/eve…)
When presenting to colleagues, look at the camera, not your screen!
Simple thing: clean your camera and/or invest in a new one - you can plug in an external one.
I've been reading Next Generation Performance Management today by @acolquitt - he reviews and summarises evidence on how to manage performance in ways that work, not based on "myth and superstition". (Thread)
Key message 1: most efforts to base PM on performance ratings fail, across all industries. It's not clear that they're useful in raising performance in any case.
Most organisations are convinced that financial rewards are their most important motivator of performance. This doesn't seem backed up by evidence - would appear more effective to focus on development and team relationships.
I'm having a bit of a Cognitive Load Theory day today. I want to highlight a few findings from this 2011 paper by Paas and Sweller (thread) link.springer.com/article/10.100…
Pupils often learn better when teachers make gestures while explaining an idea, when compared to verbal-only explanations.
But it's not just teachers who benefit. Pupils who mimic and repeat taught gestures seem to learn more.
Firstly, this seems to underline why collaborative enquiry (eg Lesson Study) may be such an effective model for teachers, but also helps us understand the plausible boundary conditions.
It's effective because problem-solving in the domain of classroom pedagogy is so complex that a combined, larger collective working memory could be very beneficial.