We had our department's doctoral students' meeting today. High time to share more about the topic I'm working on for my dissertation!

All please welcome #evolution of #altruism!
Put simply, altruism means unconditionally helping another. You might think it's a human trait, but it's actually present in lots of animal species! Let's have a look at some.

In the previous tweet, you could see an example of guarding against predators. Is that altruism?
Keeping guard - called sentinel behavior - makes you more exposed and vulnerable to predators, while it protects others in your group.

Is there a benefit for the sentry?
We'll get to that! Meerkat sentry. Credit: Emily P. Robinson, Fight for Wildlif
Other examples include sharing food, aiding the injured, helping at others' nest, nursing others' offspring... For instance, this behavior, called allofeeding, was observed in zebras. Why does it happen? Zebra mare feeding a colt
Satiated vampire bats routinely share blood with those who have gone hungry that night. Bats have fast metabolism and can't survive hungry for too long, so this food sharing is a great aid. What's responsible for the origin of this behavior?
Some of you replying to the thread already guessed a part of the answer, but it's only a part of it, not the full picture. Let's look at the benefits of altruism.

Yes, benefits: in the evolutionary scope, there need to be some for the behavior to persist

To avoid confusion: psychology and evolutionary biology view altruism through different lenses. Psychology sees an individual trying to sincerely help. Biology looks for the benefits. Yet both are complementary.

How so? Let's ask Niko Tinbergen!
This brilliant ethologist highlighted four levels of explanation for a trait - basically combinations of looking at the big or small picture, short- or long-term.

Psychology asks about the individual development of altruism. Neuropsychology would focus on the neural mechanisms.
Evolutionary biology, not surprisingly, asks the long-term, big-picture question: How is this behavior adaptive? How could it be selected for?

One answer is kin selection. Basically, it means that if you help your kin, you help pass alleles you also share to the next generation.
Kin selection appears to be responsible for a lot of acts of altruism, but by far not all! So what else happens there?

Let's look closer at the vampire bats, because experiments showed that kin selection is NOT meaningfully responsible there. vampire bat
In a famous study, Gerald Wilkinson looked at the associations of bats - who helped whom, whether they helped back, etc. - and their degree of relatedness, kinship. Helping was predicted far more by previous experience than relatedness.

nature.com/articles/30818…
Apart from observing, the author also experimented, starving a bat each night and seeing if others would help. They would... and later on, those who'd been helped were more likely to help those who'd aided them.

In short, we've got reciprocity. vampire bats
Put very simply, reciprocity could be boiled down to "I help you, you help me later". It sounds easy, and it may be for us humans... but it's actually pretty cognitively demanding!

You need to remember many other individuals, your previous interactions, and 'imagine' the future.
After William Hamilton proposed kin selection as a frequent explanation for altruism in 1964, Robert Trivers stepped in with reciprocity in 1971. Is that it, rest of mystery solved?

journals.uchicago.edu/doi/abs/10.108…
Nah! Like I said, reciprocity is cognitively demanding. Sometimes we can find other explanations. Like harrassment, which probably happens in some cases of allosuckling (nursing another's offspring). For the zebra, giraffe or other mare, it may be easier to give in to the foal.
Even the seemingly clear-cut case of vampire bats may have other influences contributing to the origin of food sharing among them, like pseudoreciprocity (helping others not because of later aid, but bc their presence is benefitting you some other way).

tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.416…
Where there's reciprocity, it need not be direct ("I help you, you help me later"). It can also be indirect ("I help someone, others see it and may be more inclined to help later").

We get reputation.

That's where the sentinels come in...
We've got a reply saying that meerkat sentinel behavior is not altruism - but that's not entirely certain. In fact, there's still no consensus over meerkats. Some studies supported the altruism option, some didn't. As to guarding and rank...

Sentinel's experience rather than rank seems to predict others' trust in the sentinel's vigilance: nature.com/articles/s4159…

Also, some tend to guard far more than others (observed in the wild as well as captivity), not entirely hierarchy-related: onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.10…
But neither of those studies answers the question of altruism. An older study found that meerkats tend to 'volunteer for guard duty' once their bellies are full and choose safe sites as lookouts. Still, is it more adaptive for them than laying low?

pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10356387/
In general, altruistic acts giving you a good reputation may not only lead to a greater chance of aid when you need it, but also show you as someone with resources and capability, a desirable partner. Which gets us to another evolutionary influence of altruism: costly signaling.
Mind you, that might not be the meerkat case, since there are other factors in play - but altruism may be one of the many cases of costly displays of one's worth as a partner, akin to a peacock's tail.

jstor.org/stable/3677205

royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/abs/10.109…
Altruism includes a complex set of behaviors - it appears to have arisen many times independently, and can't be boiled down to a single explanation. In some cases, it may be driven by kin selection; elsewhere reciprocity, costly signaling or other factors and their combinations.
So what can we do to trace its origins? We can observe animals (including people) and run experiments! It's getting late here, but hopefully we'll still have time to get to the game theory of birds and lever-pulling rats and chimps tomorrow. For now - good night, fellow humans.
We're briefly back! So how do we study what mechanisms contribute to altruism in laboratory conditions?

Often, resource division dilemmas and experimental games originally developed for studying people are used, such as the Prisoner's Dilemma, Dictator and Ultimatum Game. The Prisoner’s Dilemma: two by two matrix. Top left cell:
In the Prisoner's Dilemma, imagine two criminals caught by the police. They can each be tried for a minor crime and get a short sentence, but if one talks about a greater one, he walks free and his accomplice gets a very high sentence. If both talk, they get a medium sentence.
No matter what your accomplice does, you're better off talking: if he doesn't, you walk free. If he does, you get a medium instead of long sentence. Even though you'd collectively be better off silent, the game converges to both players talking: the so-called Nash equilibrium.
But people don't work like Nash equilibria machines and often cooperate with each other.

It also gets more interesting if you repeat the games: cooperative strategies conditional on your partner also cooperating, "tit for tat", then win in the long term.
Do animals do something similar? We can modify the game to work with pellets of food and levers or buttons, train the animals to operate the mechanism and then see what happens.

What do you think: Do blue jays cooperate in the Prisoner's Dilemma? Blue jays. Photo: Susan Kiesel/Audubon Photography Awards
In a 1995 study, blue jays didn't cooperate much in the Prisoner's Dilemma. When the game was switched to yield highest rewards for both cooperating (called mutualism), they did cooperate much more.

sciencedirect.com/science/articl…
But it might not be so simple. Food, unlike some IOU like money, is an immediate reinforcer - one tends to prefer immediate payoffs to future ones even more.

If we delay the food-based payoff, cooperation rises higher!

science.org/doi/abs/10.112…
In order to cooperate reciprocally, not just with simultaneous benefits, one needs to lower temporal discounting: the preference of immediate rewards to future, even if future ones would be higher. In many contexts, it makes sense not to wait. Waiting is also harder cognitively.
That may be the key reason why strict reciprocity seems to be rare in nature, unlike kin selection of reputation-based cooperation and altruism (where especially the latter also needs quite advanced cognition, mind you!).

But it's not the end of the story...
If we look at other birds such as zebra finches, we see results similar to the blue jays. But what about mammals? Rats, or even apes? Do you think we get more reciprocity there?
Rats exhibit generalized reciprocity: prior experience of being helped leads to greater willingness to help others.

journals.plos.org/plosbiology/ar…
Later study revealed that rats are also capable of direct reciprocity (helping the one who'd helped them), which is stronger than generalized. Yay for rats! (They're very smart and social animals.)

link.springer.com/article/10.100…
Finally, chimps; as close to humans as it can get. A 2007 study showed them helping others to food basically regardless of whether the food allocation was fair or not - whatever their own reward.

science.org/doi/abs/10.112…
However, in this experimental design, using immediate access to food upon completing the task may have influenced the result. When tokens (that the chimps were trained to exchange for food) were used, they weren't exactly enthusiastic about getting less than their colleague...
That subsequent study is here: pnas.org/content/110/6/…

However, that one had its own potential problems with the experimental design: pnas.org/content/110/33…
In nature, we can see chimps being cruel and selfish as well as helpful and selfless - depends toward whom, much like with us humans!
And with that, I'd like to wrap up the thread. Animals display a remarkable range of complex behaviors, including altruism. We're just beginning to understand its full scope and origins: and while at it, we can see even better with how wonderful creatures we share the world with!

• • •

Missing some Tweet in this thread? You can try to force a refresh
 

Keep Current with RealScientists | Women+ of Color Project

RealScientists | Women+ of Color Project Profile picture

Stay in touch and get notified when new unrolls are available from this author!

Read all threads

This Thread may be Removed Anytime!

PDF

Twitter may remove this content at anytime! Save it as PDF for later use!

Try unrolling a thread yourself!

how to unroll video
  1. Follow @ThreadReaderApp to mention us!

  2. From a Twitter thread mention us with a keyword "unroll"
@threadreaderapp unroll

Practice here first or read more on our help page!

More from @realscientists

7 Oct
I meant to tweet more actively this week, but my three-year-old's fever derailed these plans. Luckily, she's better now. This came at the same time as me starting an elimination diet to find out what's problematic for my breastfed baby.

Let's have a look at infant allergies.
This may come as a surprise to most non-parents, but food allergies are surprisingly common in infants. The estimates vary a lot, but fluctuate around 5%: one in twenty infants having a food allergy. You likely know some.

But it's hard to know for sure for reasons we'll look at.
Disclaimer: unlike evolutionary biology and astrobiology, it's not my field of study. But I've raised one child with allergies already, it looks like the younger one may have them too, and I'm planning to tackle the topic as a science writer.
Read 26 tweets
7 Oct
I mentioned the movie Europa Report in the #Europa thread yesterday. It's an amazing piece of science fiction about this moon.

But there are plenty of interesting takes on life in a subsurface ocean in SF! Let's have a look at a few...
'A Darkling Sea' by James Cambias is an interesting novel featuring a civilization in an exoplanetary subsurface ocean. Much recommended!

goodreads.com/book/show/1793…
"Innumerable Glimmering Lights", a short story by Rich Larson, has no human characters, but that doesn't mean they aren't relatable. They're the right mix of alien and familiar, and it would be interesting to see a larger piece of the world crafted there.

goodreads.com/book/show/3694…
Read 10 tweets
3 Oct
"All these worlds are yours. Except Europa. Attempt no landing there." - 2010: Odyssey Two, by A.C. Clarke

Most of you think we'll first discover life outside of Earth on Europa or Enceladus. Let's have a look at these amazing worlds of ice and water! 🧵 Europa. Image: NASA / JPL/ ...
In 1610, Europa, Io, Ganymede and Callisto were discovered almost at the same time by Simon Marius and Galileo Galilei.

We remember the latter better, but those names were selected by Marius. If not for him, we might have been calling them after the Medicis, Galileo's patrons... Galileo's drawings of Jupit...
It wasn't until the Pioneers & Voyagers in the 1970s, though, until we got a closer look at the moon and could marvel at it.

Look at this composite image captured by the Galileo probe. Does anything look strange to you? How does it differ from our Moon?

I'll give you a moment. Europa.
Read 39 tweets
2 Oct
Excellent question! The answer is partly related to planetary protection and to engineering, partly to the uncertainty of our knowledge about liquid water on Mars now and its accessibility. Thread 🧵.
I'll start with the water. While the geology of Mars - dried-up riverbeds, deltas - suggests it likely had plenty of liquid water long ago, it currently has too thin atmosphere for liquid water to stably exist on its surface, regardless of temperature. We have to go underground! Dry delta on Mars.
There probably has been volcanic activity on Mars quite recently (maybe tens of millions years ago), so there might be hydrothermal vents underground, but we don't know where.

Then there are, of course, the polar caps - and the possibility of lakes deep under the ice. Martian south polar ice cap
Read 15 tweets
2 Oct
#Astrobiology it is! 🦠 🌃 🔭

Not a surprise. Most of us have asked ourselves whether we are alone in the nearby universe, whether there is other life, and if so, whether it is microbial, macroscopic, or even intelligent...

But how can we study something we don't know exists?
Britannica defines astrobiology as a "multidisciplinary field dealing with the nature, existence, and search for extraterrestrial life" - practically spot on, but we should emphasize that for this, we study the evolution of life on Earth, extreme conditions it can survive...
Plus to learn the chances of life elsewhere, we need to know about the lifetime and 'behavior' of stars, planetary geophysics, atmospheric physics...

In short, astrobiology is a huge interdisciplinary field encompassing biology, chemistry, geology, physics, but also humanities! Rover Under the Milky Way - Atacama Rover Astrobiology Drill
Read 8 tweets
20 Aug
As wonderfully described by @CCriadoPerez in her book ‘Invisible Women’, women are living in a world designed for men. Now in an era of increased data science, that means that there is a data bias towards men (1/5)
This wonderful book covers everything medical data (including ‘standard’ symptoms of heart attack) is based on men, that women are 47% more likely to be seriously injured in a car accident and asks ‘can snow clearing can be sexist?’ (2/5)
Current research shows that female uptake for clinical trials is still low (bmj.com/content/371/bm…) and even in trials where women are recruited, lots don’t provide gender disaggregated data. (3/5)
Read 5 tweets

Did Thread Reader help you today?

Support us! We are indie developers!


This site is made by just two indie developers on a laptop doing marketing, support and development! Read more about the story.

Become a Premium Member ($3/month or $30/year) and get exclusive features!

Become Premium

Too expensive? Make a small donation by buying us coffee ($5) or help with server cost ($10)

Donate via Paypal Become our Patreon

Thank you for your support!

Follow Us on Twitter!

:(